What Is Consensual Perception and How Can We Escape It?

Apr 13th, 2015 | By | Category: Articles

ECP

(Images by Jasun Horsley)

I

Let’s start with three concepts. Consensus. Perception. Language.

  • Consensus: a general agreement about something : an idea or opinion that is shared by all the people in a group.
  • Perception: awareness of the elements of the environment through physical sensation. (This was the third definition I found online: the first one, interestingly enough, was: “the way you think about or understand someone or something.” Keep this in mind, I will refer back to it.)
  • Language: the system of words or signs that people use to express thoughts and feelings to each other.

The general agreement is that both language and consensus are shaped by perception, i.e., what we perceive dictates the terms of communication and what we can agree about.

I am on the autism spectrum, making me a perceptual anomaly who rarely agrees with other people about what’s perceptibly real. I cannot agree with the above. From my point of view, it’s the exact opposite: language and consensus determine what we (allow ourselves to) perceive. (I should probably extend the idea of “language,” however, to include the entire spectrum of learned values and meanings.)

So right out the gate I’m faced with a seeming infinite regress of problems: How to use language to demonstrate that language prevents us from grokking certain concepts, including the concept that language prevents us from grokking . . . ; and so on, ad infinitum.

My first attempt at this piece was for a fairly mainstream writer’s website. It was laid out in what I thought were clear descriptive terms, but I was told by the writer that “Most readers aren’t able to deal with numerous abstract concepts, they need concrete images.” This led to a small epiphany. Being on the autistic spectrum, I just didn’t get how many of the concepts I was describing might be “abstract” to other people, because for me, they are quite concrete (i.e., experientially real).

I realized that you can’t show a fish what water is. You have to lead him onto the land so he can see what it’s not. (How’s that for a concrete image?)

Talking about things that people are unable to perceive because they don’t have the language—or the consensus—to think about them is like throwing a sheet over an invisible man. You can’t help them see it; you can only make them aware that something is there. (Concrete image number 2.)

 

II

ECP2

Imagine a child who encounters some form of sentience that its parents can’t perceive. What do the parents do? They call it an “imaginary friend.” They don’t want to entertain the possibility that some weird phenomena is interacting with their child (scary), much less that their child is diagnosably schizophrenic (scarier). They have consensually agreed on the language construct “imaginary friend,” based not on any perception (you can’t perceive it, because it’s imaginary!), but only on a thinking process. But if you did a survey of parents asking them what “imaginary friend” actually means, you would receive many different answers. The child, for its part, may take on the meaning of “imaginary friend” before it fully understands it. It literally can’t think outside the parameters of the language being given to it, because “thought,” from a neurotypical-adult perspective, is contingent on language.

This is what is sometimes known, in colloquial English, as “a cluster fuck.” This is also probably why perception is defined, by Merriam-Webster’s online dictionary, as primarily a matter of the way we think.

I found the afore-mentioned writer’s admonition to me that “most readers need concrete images” somewhat ironic, because I had already described, in the first draft of this piece, how some autistics “think in pictures.” Maybe other autistics think in colors, numbers (Daniel Tammet), or musical notes. Myself, I think in words, but my thought tends to veer into what neurotypicals may see as highly abstract constructions—what I like to call “arguments for the impossible.”

It’s likely that, the further from language-based thought an autistic person veers, the less likely they will be able to verbalize their experience. Many autistics don’t speak, and of those who have learned spoken language, many appear to have done so without internalizing it (taking the language implant, or “word virus,” as William Burroughs called it) to the same degree.[1]

To give a concrete example, Stephen Wiltshire is an autistic painter with the ability to recreate complex cityscapes from memory after only a single sighting. Presumably, Wiltshire isn’t thinking in language—or perceiving-as-thinking. His perceptions appear to have gone straight into the body, unmediated by language, and to come back up into consciousness the same way—as if he was pulling up a file from a hard drive.

This is not how the neurotypical brain functions, and the perceptual social consensus we exist in is generally not conducive to such alternate forms of perception. If autism entails a more liberated or “extra-consensual” perception, one result is a reduced ability to function within the consensus—the inability to communicate verbally being an obvious example. As an autistic author, I may be able to boldly go where no mind has gone before; but when it comes to communicating my experience in a way that “most readers can deal with” it—that’s a whole other kettle of fish.

To lead the fish away from water (mixing my metaphors now) means jumping in at the deep end of consensus. Since I am an air-breathing autistic, the chances I will drown before I can get anyone to “read my signals” are distressingly high. I only hope this image sufficiently illustrates the urgency of my argument.

Consensual perception not only prevents alternate forms of perception. It literally imprisons—and kills—many extra-consensual perceivers. When I write, I am in a sense writing for my life.

 

III

ECP3

Consensual perception depends on the agreement, not only about what is perceptibly real, but also to ignore, refute, or dismiss anything that challenges that agreement. When parents tell a child that the invisible agency it is mysteriously interacting with is “imaginary,” they dismiss the possibility of an aspect of reality interacting with their child that they are unable to perceive. It’s necessary for the child to go along with this interpretation, eventually, in order not to come into (what may feel like) potentially life-threatening conflict with its parents.

This process is known as socialization—or simply “growing up.” It’s what I suspect many of us feel we never quite managed to get right.

What if the child’s playmate was not imaginary but belonged to another perceptual frame of reference? Then perception has been reduced, via language, in order to fit into the consensual perceptual framework. It is a kind of imaginary murder.

Have you ever considered how the only time we ask “Did you see/hear that?” is when we doubt the reliability of our senses? We don’t generally crosscheck our experience of reality with others until some anomaly arises, because we just assume we all perceive the same things. It’s only when we question the agreement that we start to discover we don’t.

You are not reading the same piece that I am writing. It’s impossible for you to do that, ever, short of direct brain to brain transference. The reason it’s impossible is that your own internal framework alters the text via the act of reading it. All we can agree on here is that no agreement is possible.

It’s a lonely truism that all we can ever know with absolute certainty are our own perceptions (this, and the fact that we are perceiving). If you’ve ever argued with a lover or friend over the color of an item of clothing—or anything else where the subjectivity of perception comes horribly to the fore—you know how frustrating, even despair-inducing, failing to find confirmation about the reality of your perceptions can be.

Suppose you could see the sky through my eyes and what it looked like to you was what you have always called green? Words would have fooled us into thinking there was a consensus when, really, no such thing is possible. Words impose the idea of agreement by concealing the reality that we can only perceive our own perceptions.

The agreement of consensus is closer to an agreement to ignore this inescapable fact—the impossibility of consensus—and act as if we can agree. Social functioning depends on this agreement, just as the movie business does. It’s called “suspension of disbelief.”[2]

(Part Two)

Jasun Horsley is the author of Seen and Not Seen: Confessions of a Movie Autist. His website is auticulture.com

****

[1] According to research reported by Joseph Chilton Pearce in Magical Child, spoken language installs itself in a child’s body as it is learned. Pearce cites studies showing how “so-called random movements immediately coordinated with speech when speech was used around the infants.” These and subsequent studies further revealed that “each infant had a complete and individual repertoire of body movements that synchronized with speech: that is, that each had a specific muscular response to each and every part of his culture’s speech pattern.” By adulthood, Pearce added, “the movements have become microkinetic, discernible only by instrumentation, but nevertheless clearly detectable and invariant. The only exception found was in autistic children, who exhibited no such body-speech patterning.” This suggests an internalization of something external, and may be direct evidence of how language installs itself into human consciousness as thought—internal dialogue.

[2] As a general rule, we will do anything to avoid the feeling of being alone in our perceptions. In fact, our survival once depended on agreement: which plants were edible, which wildlife to be avoided, and so on. In our individual experience, we learned to agree with our parents’ version of reality in order to be able to communicate our needs properly and have them be met. Once again, the question of what is “perceptually incorrect” is more than just a philosophical one. It’s a socio-political reality that affects not just our chances for success, but our chances for survival.

12 Comments to “What Is Consensual Perception and How Can We Escape It?”

  1. And so, if I agree with your argument, Jasun, can we then agree that it is unlikely that we have agreed to a singular argument?

    This has elements of the superstring theory of physics, within which matter may have the ability to explore all possible energy permutations. The ones we perceive are not the totality of what is potentially perceivable. Are imaginary friends the ability to perceive some of those alternative strings of matter? Seth discussed this in Jane Robert’s Seth books, in particular “The Nature of Personal Reality: A Seth Book” , and the “The Unknown Reality: A Seth Book.”

    It would seem that at the core of this argument is that the heart of the reality of existence is that it is a personal experience, but one that is tempered and enriched by the personal experiences of others.

    Thus it comes down to perception and awareness. And how our ability to perceive is constricted by our language constructs and expectations. And how our awareness is constricted by ability to perceive.

    The stinker is, is there within whatever it is that life is an awareness of us and within it an energy to expand our perceptions and awareness?

    • The stinker is
      is there within
      whatever it is that life is
      an awareness of us
      and within it
      an energy to expand
      our perceptions and awareness?

      I turned it into a poem in the hope the question becomes an answer…

  2. triggerhappydelusion says:

    Nicely explained. Sometimes it feels like you put words to thoughts I found hard to put words to myself.

    Though I am on the Autisitc spectrum aswell and never found your texts incoherent or “too abstract to comprehend” in the first place.

  3. anthony w lowman says:

    Very interesting article! I’m looking forward to reading more. I have found myself, at times, trying to describe to college students some of the same concepts you’ve discussed so well right here.

    Thanks for drawing attention to an area all of us know we should be considering.

  4. Kira says:

    Well done. I am a twin and some of my memories of infancy are his. For instance, I have memories of wearing braces on my legs while trying to sleep in a crib. I never wore braces on my legs but my brother did and I learned this by sharing my memories with my mother. My brother also invented his own language for which I was the sole interpreter, until he agreed to speak the agreed upon language (English). My son also invented his own language for which both me and his father were the interpreters, until he also agreed to speak the agreed upon language (English). I have been learning Native languages for 20 years (Lakota, Potawatomi and Choctaw). It is true that language can limit perception and it is also true that perception can limit language and that language can widen perspective as well as narrow perspective. All these things are true.

    • I read recently, in Subliminal Seduction (1974) that science has discovered over thirty different senses of which humans are capable, yet somehow we are still stuck with the standard 5 descriptors (and a fuzzy “6th” sense)….

  5. Greg Tramel says:

    yep

  6. Stephen says:

    Quality article. You’re absolutely correct about the limitations of language in reference to the self. Specifically thinking with language. From some psychedelic use my thoughts and words don’t synchronize as they used to. This makes things harder to abstract and “think about” but I can think about things in an energetic way instead. I can look at something and a formless thought will come from an internal intention but the words normally associated with the thought (which are usually on top of the thought/ take credit for the thought) stray along top of it awkwardly.

    Since this, I’ve been thinking in pictures as you say. I used to think only in words. There may be innumerable ways to integrate the mind with thoughts outside of the standard perception.

    Another thought: Don Juan speaks to Carlos Casteneda about the necessity of drugs on the sorcerers path. He says that drugs are not normally necessary;however, for Carlos they were since his perceptions were different than that of the Indians. Maybe this perceptual norm is the same word based one you speak of here?

    • “They do untold harm to the body” was what CC wrote/DJ said about drugs. And it was because Carlos was “stupid.”

      Since (if?) CC died of liver cancer, that might have been a miscalculation on the part of “don Juan”?

      In his last book, CC spoke about “the flyers” using syntax to possess and dominate us….

  7. A. Byrd says:

    I’m in agreement not to agree I think….so what do I do for my autistic students?

  8. ciaran mac cormaic says:

    l think you are all disappearing up your own arses here. Lets all get back to basics , we are source code.
    Revisit the handbook matrix warrior for clarification.
    Best Wishes, Ciaran.

Leave a Comment